Bee Navigation: Bumblebees Find Way To Flowers Using Trial And Error, Study Shows

Tiny Sensors Reveal Secret Of Bee Navigation
Open Image Modal

By Katherine Harmon

Bumblebees, it turns out, don’t bumble. Using tiny radar tracking devices, motion-activated cameras and artificial flowers, scientists have learned how the bees themselves quickly learn the best routes to take when they go foraging from flower to flower. In fact, their cognitive competence in this area seems to match that of bigger-brained animals.

A team of researchers from Queen Mary University of London outfitted a colony of buff-tailed bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) with miniscule harmonic radio sensors and plastic number tags. They trained the bees to feed on artificial flowers that offered a perch and a sugar solution in the center. The colony’s nest box, positioned in a large field on a British estate, was then situated near five of these artificial flowers. The flowers were arranged in a pentagon shape, with each one 50 meters from the next, which is many times the distance a bumblebee can see. That arrangement prevented the subjects from following each other or spotting the next flower. The “flowers” were watched by motion-sensing video cameras to capture each bee’s feeding. The researchers also chose to complete the experiment in October, when local flowers would have faded and not tempt the bees away from the experimental ones.

“Initially, their routes were long and complex, revisiting empty flowers several times,” Mathieu Lihoreau, of the university’s School of Biological and Chemical Sciences and co-author on the study, said in a prepared statement. “But, as they gained experience, the bees gradually refined their routes.” And they did so quickly. After only an average of 26 outings, the bees had tried only about 20 of the 120 different possible foraging routes—and reduced their total flight distance by roughly 80 percent.

Previous studies had shown similar learning curves in smaller areas the laboratory, but this was the first to demonstrate it in a more realistic scale the wild. The bees in this wild-scenario actually outperformed their lab counterparts, who only improved to about 75 percent of the optimal feeding flight. The findings were published online September 20 in PLoS Biology.

The learning appeared to be primarily through trial and error. “Each time a bee tried a new route it increased its probability of re-using the new route if it was shorter than the shortest route it had tried before,” Lihoreau said. “Otherwise, the new route was abandoned and another route was tested.”

These findings are a new cognitive laurel for these impressive insects. “The speed at which they learn through trial and error is quite extraordinary,” Lihoreau’s colleague Lars Chittka, said in a prepared statement. “This complex behavior was thought to be one which only larger-brained animals were capable of.” The study does not explore whether bees were developing mental maps of the foraging area, as humans, sharks and even, arguably, octopuses do. But simply using heuristics seems to get them pretty far (by cutting down massively on time spent buzzing through the air).

Although this navigation seems to be a no-brainer for the bees, we humans needed mathematical algorithms to analyze and understand the elegance of their behavior. “This is a really exciting result because it shows that seemingly complex behaviors can be described by relatively simple rules, which can be described mathematically,” Chris Rawlings, head of Computation and Systems Biology at Rothamsted Research, said in a prepared statement. “This means we can now use mathematics to inform us when been behavior might be affected by their environment and to assess, for example, the impact of changes in the landscape.”

The bees, themselves, seemed resistant to change, especially if it meant a reduction in especially fruitful destinations. “If we removed a flower, bees continued looking at that location—even if it was empty for an extended period of time,” Chittka said. “It seems bees don’t easily forget a fruitful flower.”

Before You Go

Most Fearsome Tiny Creatures
Assassin bug(01 of10)
Open Image Modal
Triatoma infestans Look out for it in: Mexico, Central and South AmericaWhy you should fear it: Assassin bugs transmit Chagas disease, a long-term, chronic disease that can ultimately cause serious cardiac and digestive problems. Notorious victim: Charles Darwin met one on his first trip to Argentina.
Fire caterpillar(02 of10)
Open Image Modal
Lonomia obliqua Look out for it in: Brazil, Argentina, and neighboring countriesWhy you should fear it: The caterpillars release a powerful toxin that can cause internal bleeding and massive organ failure.Notorious victim: A young Canadian tourist walked barefoot through a resort and stepped on five. Although local hospitals carried an antivenin, she didn't seek treatment until she returned home--a mistake that cost her her life. (credit:Flickr/Terra Vermelha)
Biting midge(03 of10)
Open Image Modal
Culicoides spp. Look out for it in: Everywhere.Why you should fear it: Also called no-see-ums, biting midges are a serious annoyance in the Scottish Highlands--so much so that tourists check the Biting Midge Forecast before heading out for a round of golf or a trek to a distillery. In Brazil and around the Amazon, they transmit Oropouche fever.Notorious victim: According to a community study, the biting midge broke up marriages in Hervey Bay, Australia, presumably because couples were forced to spend more time indoors together.
Paederus beetle (04 of10)
Open Image Modal
Paederus sp. Look out for it in: Most of the world. Why you should fear it: The beetle lands on the skin but doesn't bite. People tend to want to slap it, which releases a nasty poison called pederin that causes horrible blisters and welts.Notorious victim: Our troops stationed in Iraq. The beetles tend to swarm around the bright lights at military bases.
Asian giant hornet(05 of10)
Open Image Modal
Vespa mandarina japonica Look out for it in: Japan, China, Taiwan, KoreaWhy you should fear it: Stings deliver a powerful neurotoxin that could be fatal.Notorious victim: Dr. Masato Ono, the world's leading expert on the giant hornet, said the sting felt like "a hot nail through my leg."
Pork tapeworm(06 of10)
Open Image Modal
Taenia solium Look out for it in: South America, Africa, Asia, and parts of Europe and North AmericaWhy you should fear it: While modern livestock management here at home has practically eliminated tapeworm-infested pork, the tapeworm eggs can be spread directly from one infected person to another. How? Let's just say that it's really, really important to wash hands after going to the bathroom-- and leave it at that.Notorious victim: A woman in Arizona went into surgery thinking she had a brain tumor, and woke up later to learn that the cause of her problems had been a tapeworm, not a tumor. (credit:Flickr/kat m research)
Deer tick (07 of10)
Open Image Modal
Ixodes scapularis Look out for it in: Eastern United States (other species that transmit Lyme are found in the West and in Europe)Why you should fear it: The nymphs transmit the miserable and difficult-to-treat Lyme diseaseNotorious victim: Polly Murray, a resident of Lyme, Connecticut, battled the disease for decades and led the fight to get it properly identified, diagnosed, and treated.
Chigoe flea(08 of10)
Open Image Modal
Tunga penetrans Look out for it in: Tropical beaches in Latin America, the Caribbean, India, and Africa.Why you should fear it: Tiny fleas burrow under toenails and lay eggs, creating awful sores and possible infectionNotorious victim: Members of Christopher Columbus' crew were made so miserable by chigoe fleas that they cut off their own toes to get rid of the bugs.
Scorpion (09 of10)
Open Image Modal
Centruroides sp. Look out for it in: Southern United States, Central and South AmericaWhy you should fear it: The venom can cause severe pain, difficulty breathing, and can be fatal to small children.Notorious victim: A little boy vacationing with his family in Mexico stepped on a scorpion in his shoe. He was flown to a hospital in San Diego, placed on life support, and did survive. (credit:Flickr/Furryscaly)
Bed bugs(10 of10)
Open Image Modal
Cimex lectulariusLook out for it in: Your bedWhy you should fear it: After hearing about all these other nasty creatures, you aren't still worried about bed bugs, are you? Bed bugs may be annoying, but they are not known to transmit disease. They may cause a dreadful allergic reaction, but you'll survive. Bed bugs have always been around; overuse of toxic pesticides drove them away for a few decades, but fortunately, we now realize that the chemicals were far more dangerous than the bugs.Notorious victim: You.